Mind-wandering is a common cognitive process where attention shifts from the current task to unrelated thoughts or internal experiences. It’s often spontaneous and can be either positive or negative.
Rumination is a form of repetitive negative thinking, typically focused on personal problems or past experiences. It’s often associated with depression and anxiety.
These processes may be related, as mind-wandering can sometimes lead to rumination, especially when negative thoughts arise during mind-wandering episodes.
Understanding this relationship is crucial for mental health research and interventions.

Xu, E. P., Li, J., Zapetis, S. L., Trull, T. J., & Stange, J. P. (2024). The mind wanders to dark places: Mind-wandering catalyzes rumination in the context of negative affect and impulsivity. Emotion, 24(8), 1826–1836. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001397
Key Points
- Mind-wandering predicts increased subsequent rumination in everyday life.
- Contextual factors like heightened negative affect and momentary impulsivity strengthen the relationship between mind-wandering and subsequent rumination.
- Depression history did not significantly moderate the relationship between mind-wandering and rumination.
- Individuals with remitted major depressive disorder (MDD) showed higher levels of mind-wandering and negative affect compared to healthy volunteers.
- The study has limitations such as the potential conflation of spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering, and the need for more frequent measurements within shorter time intervals.
- This research highlights the importance of understanding the dynamics between mind-wandering and rumination in the context of mental health and cognitive processes.
Rationale
Previous research has established rumination as a major cognitive risk factor for major depressive disorder (MDD) (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008; Spasojević & Alloy, 2001).
While the role of rumination in MDD is well-established, the potential contribution of other forms of task-unrelated thought (TUT), such as mind-wandering, to MDD remains less clear.
Recent work has proposed that mind-wandering may gradually transition into rumination, particularly in certain contexts like high negative affect (Amir & Bernstein, 2022; Chaieb et al., 2022; Hoffmann et al., 2016; Kornacka et al., 2022; Marchetti et al., 2016). This transition could subsequently contribute to the risk for MDD.
Understanding whether and under what circumstances mind-wandering leads to rumination is crucial for identifying proximal risk factors for rumination and developing targeted interventions to prevent ruminative episodes.
The next step in this line of research is to investigate the temporal relationships between mind-wandering and rumination in everyday life, examining both contextual factors (e.g., intensity of negative affect, momentary impulsivity) and individual factors (e.g., MDD history) as potential moderators of these relationships.
Method
The study employed ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to collect intensively sampled data from young adults with remitted MDD and healthy volunteers.
Participants completed surveys on mind-wandering, rumination, negative affect, and momentary impulsivity multiple times a day for 7 days.
Procedure
Participants were trained on the EMA protocol and asked to complete six surveys each day for 7 days.
Surveys were sent in pairs, with a “pre” survey sent each morning, afternoon, and evening, followed by a “post” survey 30 minutes after each “pre” survey was completed.
Surveys were sent on a fixed schedule over the course of 11 hours each day.
Sample
The study included 44 young adults with remitted MDD and 38 healthy volunteers. Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 40 years old.
The sample was diverse in terms of race and ethnicity, with 42.68% identifying as White, 14.63% as Black, 10.98% as Latine, 20.73% as Asian, 9.76% as multiracial, and 1.22% as another race.
Measures
- Rumination: Adapted items from the Ruminative Responses Scale and Momentary Ruminative Self-Focus Inventory
- Mind-wandering: Two items adapted from previous EMA studies
- Negative Affect: Modified, brief version of the Positive and Negative Affect Scale
- Momentary Impulsivity: Momentary Impulsivity Scale
Statistical measures
The researchers used multilevel models with restricted maximum likelihood estimation.
They disentangled between-person and within-person effects by separating Level 1 predictors into Level 2 (person-mean) and Level 1 (person-mean centered) variables.
Random slopes and a continuous autoregressive error structure were specified to account for individual differences and unequal time intervals between surveys.
Results
Hypothesis 1: Mind-wandering predicts increased subsequent rumination.
Result: Supported. At both between-person and within-person levels, higher mind-wandering was associated with greater subsequent rumination.
Hypothesis 2: Rumination predicts increased subsequent mind-wandering.
Result: Not supported. While rumination was associated with mind-wandering at the between-person level, it did not predict subsequent mind-wandering at the within-person level.
Hypothesis 3: Depression history moderates the relationship between mind-wandering and subsequent rumination.
Result: Not supported. Depression history did not significantly moderate this relationship.
Hypothesis 4: Negative affect strengthens the relationship between mind-wandering and subsequent rumination.
Result: Supported. In moments of higher negative affect, the relationship between mind-wandering and subsequent rumination was stronger.
Hypothesis 5: Momentary impulsivity strengthens the relationship between mind-wandering and subsequent rumination.
Result: Supported. In moments of higher momentary impulsivity, the relationship between mind-wandering and subsequent rumination was stronger.
Insight
This study provides empirical support for the notion that mind-wandering may transition into rumination in everyday life.
The findings suggest that spontaneous mind-wandering can be maladaptive, particularly in contexts involving heightened negative affect and momentary impulsivity.
Interestingly, individuals with remitted MDD and healthy individuals did not differ in their tendency to ruminate in response to mind-wandering, suggesting a potentially dimensional and transdiagnostic nature of this relationship.
The study extends previous research by elucidating how contextual features influence the relationship between spontaneous mind-wandering and rumination at the within-person level.
It highlights the importance of considering both individual and contextual factors when examining cognitive processes related to depression risk.
Future research could focus on longitudinal studies to clarify whether mind-wandering is a vulnerability factor or a psychological consequence of MDD.
Additionally, investigating whether individuals with active MDD show stronger associations between mind-wandering and rumination compared to those with remitted MDD and healthy individuals could provide insights into state-dependent features of depression.
Implications
The findings suggest that interventions focused on reducing spontaneous mind-wandering, such as mindfulness meditation training, could potentially decrease not only mind-wandering but also subsequent rumination and vulnerability to depression.
Just-in-time adaptive interventions delivered during moments of heightened mind-wandering could be effective in promoting mindfulness instead of rumination.
These interventions could be particularly beneficial when delivered during moments of increased negative affect or impulsivity.
For clinical practice, the results highlight the importance of addressing mind-wandering as a potential risk factor for rumination and depression.
Clinicians might consider incorporating strategies to reduce mind-wandering or to help individuals manage their thoughts more effectively when mind-wandering occurs.
Future research could explore the potential benefits and drawbacks of reducing mind-wandering, considering its possible role in creative problem-solving and other cognitive processes.
Additionally, investigating the efficacy of interventions targeting mind-wandering in preventing or reducing depressive symptoms could be a fruitful avenue for future studies.
Strengths
The study had many methodological strengths including:
- Use of ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to capture real-time experiences in participants’ daily lives
- Inclusion of both individuals with remitted MDD and healthy volunteers
- Examination of both between-person and within-person effects
- Investigation of multiple potential moderators (depression history, negative affect, impulsivity)
- Use of sophisticated statistical analyses (multilevel modeling)
- Consideration of both trait-level and state-level factors influencing the relationship between mind-wandering and rumination
Limitations
The study has several limitations:
- The sample was limited to young adults, potentially limiting generalizability across the lifespan.
- The study did not directly distinguish between spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering, which may have different relationships with rumination.
- The time interval between measurements (approximately 4 hours) may have been too long to capture the immediate transition from mind-wandering to rumination.
- The sample size may have been underpowered to detect small effect sizes, particularly for group differences.
- The study did not examine how symptoms of anxiety or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, which have been associated with mind-wandering, may impact the relationship between mind-wandering and rumination.
These limitations suggest that future research should include a more diverse age range, explicitly distinguish between types of mind-wandering, use more frequent measurements with shorter intervals, employ larger sample sizes, and consider a broader range of potential moderating factors and comorbid conditions.
References
Primary reference
Xu, E. P., Li, J., Zapetis, S. L., Trull, T. J., & Stange, J. P. (2024). The mind wanders to dark places: Mind-wandering catalyzes rumination in the context of negative affect and impulsivity. Emotion, 24(8), 1826–1836. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001397
Other references
Amir, I., & Bernstein, A. (2022). Dynamics of internal attention and internally-directed cognition: The attention-to-thoughts (A2T) model. Psychological Inquiry, 33(4), 239-260. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2141000
Chaieb, L., Hoppe, C., & Fell, J. (2022). Mind wandering and depression: A status report. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 133, 104505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.12.028
Hoffmann, F., Banzhaf, C., Kanske, P., Bermpohl, F., & Singer, T. (2016). Where the depressed mind wanders: Self-generated thought patterns as assessed through experience sampling as a state marker of depression. Journal of affective disorders, 198, 127-134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.03.005
Kornacka, M., Atzeni, T., Krejtz, I., Bortolon, C., & Baeyens, C. (2022). Task Unrelated Thoughts (TUT) affecting mood in ecological settings: from adaptive mind-wandering to maladaptive rumination. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 44, No. 44).
Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H., Klinger, E., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Spontaneous thought and vulnerability to mood disorders: The dark side of the wandering mind. Clinical psychological science, 4(5), 835-857. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702615622383
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on psychological science, 3(5), 400-424. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x
Spasojević, J., & Alloy, L. B. (2001). Rumination as a common mechanism relating depressive risk factors to depression. Emotion, 1(1), 25. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.25
Keep Learning
Socratic questions for a college class to discuss this paper:
- How might the relationship between mind-wandering and rumination differ across different age groups or developmental stages?
- What are the potential benefits of mind-wandering, and how might reducing it impact these positive aspects?
- How could cultural differences influence the relationship between mind-wandering and rumination?
- What other contextual factors, beyond negative affect and impulsivity, might influence the transition from mind-wandering to rumination?
- How might the findings of this study inform the development of digital mental health interventions?
- What ethical considerations should be taken into account when designing interventions to reduce mind-wandering?
- How might the relationship between mind-wandering and rumination be different in individuals with other mental health conditions, such as anxiety disorders or ADHD?
- What neurobiological mechanisms might underlie the transition from mind-wandering to rumination?
- How could the findings of this study be applied to educational settings to support student mental health and academic performance?
- What role might social media and technology use play in the relationship between mind-wandering and rumination in daily life?
