Yerkes-Dodson Law of Arousal and Performance

The Yerkes–Dodson law is a psychology principle stating that performance improves with mental arousal – like excitement or alertness – up to an optimal point. Beyond that point, too much arousal causes performance to drop. Think of it as an inverted-U curve: low arousal leads to boredom and poor results, moderate arousal boosts focus and efficiency, and excessive arousal leads to stress and mistakes.

yerkes dodson law
The concept of optimal arousal in relation to performance on a task is depicted here. Performance is maximized at the optimal level of arousal, and it tapers off during under- and overarousal.

Key Takeaways

  • Definition: The Yerkes–Dodson law is a psychology principle showing that performance improves with arousal up to an optimal point, after which it declines. This relationship is often illustrated as an inverted-U curve.
  • Optimal zone: Moderate arousal—neither too low nor too high—tends to produce the best results. Low arousal can cause boredom, while high arousal can lead to stress and errors.
  • Task influence: The ideal arousal level depends on task difficulty. Simple tasks may benefit from higher arousal, while complex or unfamiliar tasks require lower arousal for peak performance.
  • Real-world use: The principle helps explain performance patterns in areas like sports, test-taking, public speaking, and workplace productivity. Recognizing your optimal arousal can guide preparation and stress management.
  • Limitations: The law is a general guideline, not a strict rule. Individual differences, situational factors, and modern research show that the curve can vary across people and contexts.

How the Law Works

The Yerkes–Dodson law describes the relationship between arousal and performance, showing that a moderate level of mental or physical alertness produces the best results.

In psychology, arousal refers to a general state of readiness and alertness – driven by factors like excitement, focus, or mild stress – not just feeling nervous or anxious.

Performance improves as arousal rises, but only up to an optimal point—after that, too much arousal causes it to drop.

This is known as the inverted-U model.

The curve’s shape depends on the task: complex or unfamiliar tasks are best tackled with lower arousal, while simple or well-practised tasks often benefit from higher arousal.

For example, a little pre-exam tension can sharpen focus, but overwhelming anxiety can make you forget what you know.

Yerkes Dodson Curve and Task Performance

Practical Examples

How can students use it to improve exam performance?

  • Find your sweet spot. If you feel flat (sleepy, unfocused), briefly raise arousal: stand and stretch, do 20–60 seconds of brisk movement, review a high-yield summary card, or set a 10–15 minute Pomodoro with a clear micro-goal.

  • Dial down when jittery. If you feel wired (racing heart, mind blanking), use 60–120 seconds of slow breathing (4–6 breaths/min, longer exhale), progressive muscle relaxation, or the reframe “I’m excited” to channel arousal into focus.

  • Match arousal to task. Use higher arousal for quick recall and practice tests; use lower arousal (quiet, steady pace) for complex problem-solving and multi-step calculations.

  • Exam-day routine. 5–10 minutes pre-test: light movement → two calming breaths → skim a confidence list (3 things you know cold) → pick a “first easy win” question to lock in momentum.

How does it apply in sports, public speaking, or workplace productivity?

  • Sports. Explosive, well-learned skills (sprinting, powerlifting) usually benefit from higher arousal (hype music, dynamic warm-up, energizing cue words). Fine-motor or novel/complex skills (golf putt, gymnastics routine, new play) often need lower arousal (slower breathing, quiet focus, consistent pre-shot routine).

  • Public speaking. Aim for medium arousal: convert nerves into energy with brisk walking and power posture, then settle with two slow breaths. Use a simple opening script, anchor points on your outline, and a tempo cue (“slow and clear”) to avoid rushing.

  • Workplace. Routine tasks (inbox triage, filing) can tolerate higher arousal and short sprints. Deep work (analysis, writing, coding) needs lower arousal: silence notifications, time-box 25–50 minutes, set a single outcome, and start with a 60-second calming reset.

What are everyday signs you’re over- or under-aroused for a task?

  • Under-arousal (too low): Boredom, drifting attention, procrastination, slow starts, repeated rereads with nothing sticking.

  • Quick fix: Add stakes (timer, micro-deadline), stand or walk 2 minutes, switch to an easier starter task, play neutral background noise if silence feels sleepy.

  • Optimal arousal (just right): Clear goal, steady pace, present-moment focus, small errors caught quickly, time passing normally.

  • Maintain: Brief breaks before fatigue, keep the environment stable, stick to the current goal.

  • Over-arousal (too high): Racing thoughts, muscle tension, twitchy pacing, rushing, frequent mistakes, mind blanks on steps you know.

  • Quick fix: Two minutes of slow breathing with long exhales, relax shoulders/jaw, lower stimulation (quieter room, dimmer screen), break the task into smaller chunks and re-enter with the easiest step.

Links to Psychology Theories

How does it connect to stress theories like the fight-or-flight response?

  • The fight-or-flight response is a surge of sympathetic arousal; at moderate levels (often called eustress), attention sharpens and performance improves, matching the rising side of the inverted-U.

  • When arousal becomes excessive (distress), prefrontal functions like working memory and flexible thinking degrade, producing tunnel vision and more errors—the falling side of the curve.

  • Practical read: a little time pressure can help you lock in, but panic (racing heart, breathlessness, blanking) pushes you past the peak.

How does it relate to motivation theories such as Self-Determination Theory or Drive Theory?

  • Drive Theory (classic “more arousal → better performance”) fits well-learned, simple tasks; Yerkes–Dodson refines it by adding the decline at high arousal, especially for complex or novel tasks.

  • Self-Determination Theory (SDT) explains quality of motivation: autonomy, competence, and relatedness tend to keep arousal in a productive, self-regulated range; controlling pressure elevates anxiety and risks overshooting the peak.

  • In practice: autonomy-supportive study or coaching fosters steady, optimal arousal; harsh evaluation or micromanagement spikes arousal and impairs complex performance.

How does it fit with the concept of “flow” in positive psychology?

  • Flow occurs when skill and challenge are high and balanced, with clear goals and immediate feedback—subjectively “calm but energized” focus.

  • Flow typically sits near the optimal zone of the inverted-U: aroused enough for intensity, low enough in anxiety to keep precision and flexibility.

  • Key distinction: Yerkes–Dodson models a performance–arousal curve; flow describes the quality of experience. They align at the peak, but you can feel “amped” without flow, or be in flow at different absolute arousal levels depending on the task.

Research Evidence

The Yerkes–Dodson law has been interpreted in different ways since it was first proposed in 1908.

In their original paper, Robert Yerkes and John Dodson studied discrimination learning in “dancing mice.”

The mice had to choose between a white and a black box.

Entering the white box delivered a mild (non-injurious) electric shock; the black box did not.

First series (baseline task).

With very weak shocks, mice took many tries to meet the learning goal (picking correctly 10/10 times for three days in a row).

As the shock got stronger, they learned faster—until the strongest level, where learning slowed again.

If you plot shock strength vs. number of tries (where fewer tries = better), you get a U-shape.

If you flip that to overall performance (where higher = better), you get the familiar inverted-U.

This went against the authors’ simple “more stimulation = better learning” expectation.

Second series (easier task).

Yerkes and Dodson then made the choice easier by increasing the light contrast between boxes and tested five shock levels.

Now, the strongest shocks produced the fastest learning, and the weakest produced the slowest—more like a straight increase than a U-shape (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908; Teigen, 1994).

Third series (harder task).

Next, they made the choice harder by reducing the light contrast and tested four shock levels.

Here, a moderate shock (the second-weakest) led to the best learning (fewest tries), again suggesting a U-shaped link between stimulation and learning rate (Teigen, 1994).

What they took from this.

Both too little and too much stimulation can hurt learning, and the best level depends on how difficult the task is.

As tasks get harder, the optimal stimulation shifts downward toward a gentler level (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908; Teigen, 1994).


Replication Studies

Early follow-ups generally supported the idea that task difficulty changes the effect of stimulation on learning.

  • Chicks (Cole, 1911): On easy tasks, stronger shock helped; on medium difficulty, very strong shock slowed learning; on difficult tasks, strong shock led to mixed results. Cole saw this as broadly consistent with Yerkes–Dodson.

  • Kittens (Dodson, 1915): A medium shock beat a strong one on the standard task; when the task was made easier, medium and strong shocks worked about equally well, and learning overall improved as shock increased.

  • Reward strength (Dodson, 1917):  A similar U-shape appeared with rewards: moderate deprivation (e.g., mild hunger) boosted learning, but excessive deprivation made it worse.

  • Humans (Vaughn & Diserens, 1930; summarized in Young, 1936). People learned a maze best with light or medium punishment, not with none or heavy punishment—again pointing to an optimum rather than a straight line. Young concluded: for any activity, there is an optimal degree of punishment.


From Punishment–Learning to Motivation–Performance

By the 1930s–1940s, researchers began to reframe the idea.

Punishment was no longer seen as a core driver of learning (e.g., Thorndike, 1932; Skinner, 1938; Estes, 1944), and scientists distinguished learning from performance more carefully.

Mid-century accounts treated the pattern as a link between drive/arousal (motivation) and performance, noting that too much arousal can sometimes get in the way (e.g., Hilgard & Marquis, 1961).

Textbooks and reviews (e.g., Bourne & Ekstrand, 1973) popularized the inverted-U: performance is highest at moderate arousal and lower at both extremes.

Note: Early animal work often measured tries needed to learn (lower is better), which makes the graph look U-shaped. When we talk about performance level (higher is better), the same pattern appears as an inverted-U.

A stronger test came from Broadhurst (1957), who used several motivation levels and three difficulty levels with larger groups of rats.

He found that the best motivation level moved with task difficulty—higher for easy tasks and lower for hard ones—matching Yerkes and Dodson’s core claim (Broadhurst, 1957; Teigen, 1994).

He also suggested looking at individual differences (e.g., “emotionality”) that might shift where the peak sits on the curve.

Critical Evaluation

1. Situations where performance doesn’t follow the inverted-U pattern

The inverted-U is not universal; some tasks show different shapes (linear increases, linear decreases, thresholds, or plateaus).

Very simple, well-learned speeded tasks sometimes improve almost linearly as arousal rises (consistent with classic drive and social-facilitation findings).

In contrast, highly complex or novel tasks can worsen as arousal increases, producing a mostly downward slope with little or no “rising” phase (e.g., multi-step reasoning under intense time pressure).

Vigilance or monotonous monitoring tasks can also show threshold/plateau behavior, where raising arousal from very low levels helps, but further increases add little benefit.

These departures occur because arousal affects attention breadth, working memory, and motor control in task-specific ways.

Practitioners should avoid one-size-fits-all prescriptions.

Target arousal to the task: it may be optimal to increase stimulation for dull, overlearned activities but decrease it for precision or novel problem-solving. Treat the inverted-U as a heuristic starting point, not a binding rule.

2. Personality differences (introversion vs. extraversion)

Baseline arousal and reactivity vary with personality, shifting where the “optimal zone” sits on the curve.

Introverts typically have higher baseline cortical arousal and greater sensitivity to stimulation, so they may reach overload at lower levels of noise, caffeine, audience size, or time pressure.

Extraverts often have lower baseline arousal and seek stimulation, so additional intensity (livelier environment, tighter deadlines, energizing routines) can move them toward peak performance.

Traits like trait anxiety/neuroticism and sensation-seeking further tilt the curve—height, width, and peak position can all change person-to-person.

Personalize conditions: quieter rooms, longer ramps, and calming routines for many introverts or high-anxiety individuals; brisk warm-ups, stronger cues, and lively environments for many extraverts or high sensation-seekers.

In education and workplaces, build flexibility (choice of noise levels, pacing, and break structure) rather than enforcing a single “optimal” arousal recipe.

3. Methodological flaws and oversimplifications in the original work

 The 1908 Yerkes & Dodson evidence has limitations that weaken claims of a universal “law.”

The original studies used mice learning a discrimination task with electric shock as the “arousal” manipulation—conflating punishment magnitude, motivation, stress, and learning rate.

Physiological arousal wasn’t directly measured, statistical methods were rudimentary by modern standards, and generalization from a narrow animal paradigm to diverse human performances was assumed rather than demonstrated.

Later popularizations often depict a smooth inverted-U without specifying moderators (task type, skill level, personality, context) or providing precise functional forms, encouraging over-simple applications.

Treat Yerkes–Dodson as a useful framework, not a strict law.

Modern evaluation should rely on task- and person-specific data (e.g., error rates, response variability) and, where possible, physiological indicators of arousal (heart-rate variability, pupil dilation).

Interventions should be tested and iterated for the particular context instead of assumed from the generic curve.

References

Anderson, K. J., & Revelle, W. (1983). The interactive effects of caffeine, impulsivity and task demands on a visual search task. Personality and Individual Differences, 4(2), 127-134.

Bourne, L. E., & Ekstrand, B. R. (1973). Psychology: Its principles and meanings (Dryden, Hinsdale, IL).

Broadhurst, P. L. (1957). Emotionality and the Yerkes-Dodson law. Journal of experimental psychology, 54(5), 345.

Brown, W. P. (1965). The Yerkes-Dodson law repealed. Psychological reports, 17(2), 663-666.

Cole, L. W. (1911). The relation of strength of stimulus to rate of learning in the chick. Journal of Animal Behavior, 1(2), 111.

Corbett, M. (2015). From law to folklore: work stress and the Yerkes-Dodson Law. Journal of Managerial Psychology.

Dodson, J. D. (1915). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation in the kitten. Journal of Animal Behavior, 5(4), 330.

Dodson, J. D. (1917). Relative values of reward and punishment in habit formation. Psychobiology, 1(3), 231.

Estes, W. K. (1944). An experimental study of punishment. Psychological Monographs, 57(3), i.

Geen, R. G. (1984). Preferred stimulation levels in introverts and extroverts: Effects on arousal and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(6), 1303.

Gigerenzer, G., & Murray, D. J. (2015). Cognition as intuitive statistics. Psychology Press.

Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the CNS (conceptual nervous system). Psychological review, 62(4), 243.

Hilgard, E. R., & Marquis, D. G. (1961). Hilgard and Marquis” conditioning and learning.

Levitt, E. E. (2015). The psychology of anxiety.

Loftus, E., & Ketcham, K. (1991). Witness for the defense: The accused, the eyewitness, and the expert who puts memory on trial. Macmillan.

Matthews, G. (1985). The effects of extraversion and arousal on intelligence test performance. British Journal of Psychology, 76(4), 479-493.

Revelle, W., Amaral, P., & Turriff, S. (1976). Introversion/extroversion, time stress, and caffeine: Effect on verbal performance. Science, 192(4235), 149-150.

Skinner, B. F. (2019). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. BF Skinner Foundation.

Teigen, K. H. (1994). Yerkes-Dodson: A law for all seasons. Theory & Psychology, 4(4), 525-547.

Thorndike, E. L. (1932). The fundamentals of learning.

Vaughn, J., & Diserens, C. M. (1930). The relative effects of various intensities of punishment on learning and efficiency. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 10(1), 55.

Winton, W. M. (1987). Do introductory textbooks present the Yerkes-Dodson Law correctly?. American Psychologist, 42(2), 202.

Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Punishment: Issues and experiments, 27-41.

Young, P. T. (1936). Social motivation.

Key Takeaways

  • The Yerkes-Dodson law states that there is an empirical relationship between stress and performance and that there is an optimal level of stress corresponding to an optimal level of performance. Generally, practitioners present this relationship as an inverted U-shaped curve.
  • Research shows that moderate arousal is generally best; when arousal is very high or very low, performance tends to suffer (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908).
  • Robert Yerkes (pronounced “Yerk-EES”) and John Dodson discovered that the optimal arousal level depends on the complexity and difficulty of the task to be performed.
  • This relationship is known as the Yerkes-Dodson law, which holds that a simple task is performed best when
    arousal levels are relatively high, and complex tasks are best performed when arousal levels are lower.
  • The Yerkes-Dodson law’s original formulation derives from a 1908 paper on experiments in Japanese dancing mice learning to discriminate between white and black boxes using electric shocks. This research was largely ignored until the 1950s when Hebb’s concept of arousal and the “U-shaped curve” led to renewed interest in the Yerkes-Dodson law’s general applications in human arousal and performance.
  • The Yerkes-Dodson law has more recently drawn criticism for its poor original experimental design and it’s over-extrapolated scope to personality, managerial practices, and even accounts of the reliability of eyewitness testimony.

Saul McLeod, PhD

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.


Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

Charlotte Nickerson

Research Assistant at Harvard University

Undergraduate at Harvard University

Charlotte Nickerson is a graduate of Harvard University obsessed with the intersection of mental health, productivity, and design.

h4 { font-weight: bold; } h1 { font-size: 40px; } h5 { font-weight: bold; } .mv-ad-box * { display: none !important; } .content-unmask .mv-ad-box { display:none; } #printfriendly { line-height: 1.7; } #printfriendly #pf-title { font-size: 40px; }